Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Cell. Neurosci., 06 March 2019
Sec. Cellular Neurophysiology
Volume 13 - 2019 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2019.00084

Long-Lasting Response Changes in Deep Cerebellar Nuclei in vivo Correlate With Low-Frequency Oscillations

  • 1Department of Brain and Behavioral Sciences, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy
  • 2IRCCS Mondino Foundation, Pavia, Italy

The deep cerebellar nuclei (DCN) have been suggested to play a critical role in sensorimotor learning and some forms of long-term synaptic plasticity observed in vitro have been proposed as a possible substrate. However, till now it was not clear whether and how DCN neuron responses manifest long-lasting changes in vivo. Here, we have characterized DCN unit responses to tactile stimulation of the facial area in anesthetized mice and evaluated the changes induced by theta-sensory stimulation (TSS), a 4 Hz stimulation pattern that is known to induce plasticity in the cerebellar cortex in vivo. DCN units responded to tactile stimulation generating bursts and pauses, which reflected combinations of excitatory inputs most likely relayed by mossy fiber collaterals, inhibitory inputs relayed by Purkinje cells, and intrinsic rebound firing. Interestingly, initial bursts and pauses were often followed by stimulus-induced oscillations in the peri-stimulus time histograms (PSTH). TSS induced long-lasting changes in DCN unit responses. Spike-related potentiation and suppression (SR-P and SR-S), either in units initiating the response with bursts or pauses, were correlated with stimulus-induced oscillations. Fitting with resonant functions suggested the existence of peaks in the theta-band (burst SR-P at 9 Hz, pause SR-S at 5 Hz). Optogenetic stimulation of the cerebellar cortex altered stimulus-induced oscillations suggesting that Purkinje cells play a critical role in the circuits controlling DCN oscillations and plasticity. This observation complements those reported before on the granular and molecular layers supporting the generation of multiple distributed plasticities in the cerebellum following naturally patterned sensory entrainment. The unique dependency of DCN plasticity on circuit oscillations discloses a potential relationship between cerebellar learning and activity patterns generated in the cerebellar network.

Introduction

Two functional aspects of the cerebellum, that have been emphasized in turn but proved hard to reconcile, are the pronounced oscillatory dynamics (Llinas, 1988) and the role in sensorimotor learning (Marr, 1969; Albus, 1971; Ito, 1972). Key nodes in the cerebellar circuitry are the deep cerebellar nuclei (DCN). DCN convey rhythmic outputs to the motor system (Jacobson et al., 2008) and, at the same time, have been suggested to be the site of plasticity by studies using local lesions (Ohyama et al., 2003, 2006) or electrical stimulation of afferent fiber bundles (Racine et al., 1986). Multiple forms of plasticity have been reported in DCN synapses in vitro (Morishita and Sastry, 1996; Ouardouz and Sastry, 2000; Zhang et al., 2004; Zhang and Linden, 2006; Pugh and Raman, 2009) (reviewed in Hansel et al., 2001; Gao et al., 2012; D’Angelo, 2014; Mapelli et al., 2015; D’Angelo et al., 2016b) and have been proposed to play a critical role in animal associative behaviors by computational models (Medina and Mauk, 1999; Casellato et al., 2015; Antonietti et al., 2016; D’Angelo et al., 2016a). Despite this evidence, the demonstration that long-lasting changes can actually be measured in DCN in vivo and can be related to internal circuit oscillations and plasticity was still lacking.

Deep cerebellar nuclei neurons are autorhythmic (Jahnsen, 1986a,b) and receive both excitatory inputs from collaterals of mossy and climbing fibers and inhibitory inputs from Purkinje cells (PCs) (Llinas and Muhlethaler, 1988). DCN neurons respond to tactile stimulation generating discharge patterns, which reflect the combination of inhibitory and excitatory inputs (Rowland and Jaeger, 2005, 2008; Chen et al., 2010; Canto et al., 2016; Yarden-Rabinowitz and Yarom, 2017). DCN neurons send output fibers to thalamus and to various precerebellar nuclei, influencing neuronal activity both in descending systems and in the cerebral cortex (Watson et al., 2014; Gao et al., 2018). Specific pathways also connect DCN with the inferior olive (Jacobson et al., 2008) and cerebellar granular layer (Ankri et al., 2015; Gao et al., 2016). These connections form the basis for reverberating loops that have been predicted to sustain rebound excitation and oscillatory cycles (Llinas and Muhlethaler, 1988; Kistler and De Zeeuw, 2003; Marshall and Lang, 2004; Hoebeek et al., 2010; Witter et al., 2013).

In the DCN, long-term synaptic plasticity [long-term potentiation and depression (LTP and LTD)] has been identified both at excitatory and inhibitory connections in vitro. Interestingly, excitatory plasticity depended on post-inhibitory rebound bursts (Pugh and Raman, 2006) and inhibitory plasticity required co-activation of mossy fibers (Morishita and Sastry, 1996; Ouardouz and Sastry, 2000), so that plasticity at the two synapses appears to be correlated and to require precise activation sequences.

In this work, we asked whether long-lasting changes could be induced in DCN single unit responses in anesthetized mice in vivo using facial theta sensory stimulation (TSS), which proved able in previous works to induce long-lasting changes in responses recorded from the cerebellum granular layer and molecular layer (Roggeri et al., 2008; Ramakrishnan et al., 2016). TSS actually induced long-lasting changes in DCN unit responses. Interestingly, these changes were uniquely correlated with the frequency of stimulus-induced oscillations, suggesting a close relationship between oscillatory dynamics and plasticity (D’Angelo and De Zeeuw, 2009; Cheron et al., 2016) reminiscent of induction schemes identified in hippocampus and neocortex (Buzsaki, 2006; Roy et al., 2014).

Materials and Methods

Multiple single-unit recordings were performed from the fastigial nucleus of C57BL/6 mice of either sex (40.2 ± 1.8 days old; n = 51) under urethane anesthesia. Urethane was used as its anesthetic action is exerted through multiple weak effects (including a 10% reduction of NMDA, 18% reduction of AMPA and 23% enhancement of GABA-A receptor-mediated currents) (Hara and Harris, 2002) compared to ketamine or isoflurane, which act by powerfully blocking NMDA receptors (up to 80 and 60%, respectively; Hara and Harris, 2002) and could therefore severely compromise the induction of plasticity (Godaux et al., 1990; Muller et al., 1993; Bengtsson and Jorntell, 2007; Marquez-Ruiz and Cheron, 2012; Mawhinney et al., 2012). Moreover, urethane was successfully used before in similar recording conditions to investigate plasticity in the granular layer (Roggeri et al., 2008) and molecular layer (Ramakrishnan et al., 2016) of cerebellum.

All experimental protocols were conducted in accordance with international guidelines from the European Union Directive 2010/63/EU on the ethical use of animals and were approved by the ethical committee of Italian Ministry of Health (638/2017-PR; 7/2018-PR).

Surgical Procedures

Mice were deeply anesthetized with intraperitoneal injections of urethane (Sigma-Aldrich). Induction (1.3 g/kg urethane dissolved in 0.9% NaCl) was followed by booster injections (10% of the induction dose) in order to stabilize anesthesia, starting 30 min after induction and repeating 3–4 times every 30 min. The level of anesthesia was monitored by evaluating the leg withdrawal after pinching and spontaneous whisking. The animal was then placed on a custom-built stereotaxic table covered with a heating plate (HP-1M: RTD/157, Physitemp Instruments, Inc., Clifton, NJ, United States). Body temperature was monitored with a rectal probe and maintained at 36°C through a feedback controller (TCAT-2LV controller, Physitemp Instruments, Inc., Clifton, NJ, United States). The mouse head was fixed over the Bregma to a metal bar connected to a pedestal anchored to the stereotaxic table. This arrangement allowed open access to the peri-oral area for air-puff stimulation. Surgery was performed to expose the cerebellar surface: local reflexes were reduced by subcutaneous application of lidocaine (0.2 ml; Astrazeneca), then the skin and muscles were removed. Craniotomy of the occipital bone (-7.8 mm AP, +0.50 mm ML from Bregma, in order to record from the fastigial nucleus) allowed to expose the cerebellar surface over the vermis. The dura mater was carefully removed and the surface was covered with saline (NaCl 0.9%; Sigma) to prevent drying.

Single Unit Recordings in vivo

Quartz-coated platinum/tungsten fiber electrodes (1–5 MΩ) organized in a multi-electrode array (MEA) of 4 × 4, with inter-electrode distance of 100 μm (Eckhorn matrix, Thomas Recording, GmBh, Germany) were used for neuronal recordings. Recording electrodes were positioned over the vermis, ipsilateral to the air puff stimulator, and lowered perpendicularly to the surface down to a depth of 2109.1 ± 65.5 μm (n = 33). The electrophysiological signals were digitized at 25 kHz, using a 300–5000 Hz band-pass filter, amplified and stored using a RZ5D processor multi-channel workstation (Tucker-Davis Technologies, Alachua, FL, United States). DCN neurons were identified online by assessing recording depth, spontaneous activity, and stimulus-evoked responses. At the end of recordings, an electric lesion was made by injecting current through the recording electrode. The recording site was then confirmed by histological tissue processing (see below).

Sensory Stimulation

Tactile sensory stimulation was performed using air-puffs (30 ms pulses, 30–60 psi) delivered through a small tube ending with a nozzle (0.5 mm diameter) positioned 2–3 mm away from the snout area of the animal and connected to a MPPI-2 pressure injector (Applied Scientific Instrumentation, Eugene, OR, United States) (Roggeri et al., 2008; Ramakrishnan et al., 2016). While cerebellar cortical responses to skin receptive fields stimulation are organized in the so-called “fractured somatotopy” in the granular layer and in zonal or small regions in the molecular layer (Shambes et al., 1978; Kassel et al., 1984; Ekerot and Jorntell, 2001; Jörntell and Ekerot, 2002), DCN neurons have been described to respond to large portions of the body surface, both ipsi- and contra-lateral (Rowland and Jaeger, 2005). We nevertheless limited the sensory stimulation area to mouse upper lip, lower lip or whisker pad of the ipsilateral region. Following 5 min of spontaneous activity recording, low frequency stimuli (0.5 Hz) were delivered over the mouse upper lip, lower lip or whisker pad to activate the corresponding receptive fields and evoke the neuronal response (Bower and Woolston, 1983; Morissette and Bower, 1996; Vos et al., 1999; Roggeri et al., 2008; Ramakrishnan et al., 2016) (see Figure 1A). DCN single unit responses were monitored online by building peri-stimulus time histograms (PSTHs) triggered by the air-puffs. Once a responsive unit was detected, control stimuli were delivered for 20 min at 0.5 Hz, in order to characterize unit responses to tactile sensory stimulation. Then, the TSS pattern (a burst of 100 air-puffs at 4 Hz) was delivered, followed by post-induction recordings for at least 40 min at 0.5 Hz. Since the air puff has been reported to elicit a brief spike burst in the mossy fibers (Vos et al., 1999; Chadderton et al., 2004), the TSS is likely to determine short bursts repeated at 4 Hz. This pattern is known to induce plasticity in the cerebellar cortex (see Roggeri et al., 2008; Prestori et al., 2013; Ramakrishnan et al., 2016; Romano et al., 2018). In 12 recordings TSS was not delivered, monitoring the stability of responses for at least 60 min.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Extracellular recordings from DCN units in vivo. (A) Schematic representation of the main pathways activated by air puff stimulation of the peri-oral region in mice: the trigemino-cerebellar (solid red line) and thalamo-cortical-pontine (dashed red line) pathways. The region in the gray box is expanded at the right to show the main circuit elements relevant to DCN neuron activity. MI, primary motor cortex; SI, primary sensory cortex; Th, thalamus, PN, pontine nuclei; TN, trigeminal nucleus, DCN, deep cerebellar nuclei; GrC, granule cells; MLIs, molecular layer interneurons; PC, Purkinje cell; IO, inferior olive; pf, parallel fibers; mf, mossy fibers; cf, climbing fiber; t.s., tactile stimulation. MEA, multi-electrode array (Eckhorn matrix, see section “Materials and Methods” for details). (B) Toluidine blue stained coronal cerebellar slice showing the electric lesion (arrow) made by the recording electrode in the fastigial nucleus. (C) Two single-unit recordings showing a burst and a pause in response to tactile stimulation (arrow). The raster plots show the spike discharge during ∼2 s recordings and its change caused by tactile stimulation in 300 consecutive trials.

Pharmacology

In a subset of experiments, the AMPA and NMDA receptor antagonists, 100 μM NBQX (Abcam) (Guo et al., 2016) and 250 μM D-APV (Tocris Bioscience) (Zhang et al., 2017), were injected in the fastigial nucleus near the recording electrodes. APV and NBQX were added to a Krebs solution with the following composition (in mM): 120 NaCl, 2 KCl, 1.2 MgSO4, 26 NaHCO3, 1.2 KH2PO4, 2 CaCl2, and 11 glucose, equilibrated with 95% O2–5% CO2 (pH 7.4). The solution containing the drugs was pre-loaded in a pneumatic picopump (PV820, World Precision Instruments), operated through adjustable air pressure, terminating in a 35G needle, positioned using a Patch-Star micromanipulator (Scientifica, Ltd.). After 15 min of single-unit recording, the solution was injected at the rate of 1 μl/5 min. It should be noted that the injection of GABAergic antagonists in the fastigial nucleus would not help discerning the origin of the pause, as it would affect the synapses coming from both local interneurons and PCs.

Optogenetics

Adeno Associated Virus Injection and Expression

The expression of ChR2 in the cerebellar vermis was obtained through local injection of the adeno associated viral construct pAAV-hSyn-hChR2-EYFP (AAV1 serotype; Penn Vector Core, University of Pennsylvania, United States). C57BL6 mice of either sex (30 days old, n = 13) were anesthetized with 1–2% isoflurane in oxygen 100% at 0.7 L/min delivered from a gas vaporizer (Ugo Basile S.R.L., Italy) and were placed in a stereotaxic apparatus (Leica vernier stereotaxic instrument), where they constantly received isoflurane from a nose cone and had their head fixed with ear bars. Mice body temperature was constantly monitored by a heating pad connected to a rectal thermal probe (TCAT-2LV controller, Physitemp Instruments, Inc., Clifton, NJ, United States) and maintained at 36°C. After testing the absence of withdrawal reflexes, a sagittal incision on the head was performed to expose the cranium, and a burr hole was drilled to target lobule VI of cerebellar vermis 3.5 mm posterior to Lambda. The virus was loaded into a 10 μl NanoFil syringe (World Precision Instruments) that was connected to an automatic syringe pump (Ugo Basile S.R.L., Italy). The injection needle (NF35BV, 35G, World Precision Instruments) was positioned into the vermis at 300 μm depth and 0.2 μl of virus solution at a titer of 1.16813 genome copies/ml was injected at a flow rate of 0.05 μl/min. This procedure ensured a localized expression of ChR2 at the level of cerebellar molecular and PC layers. A good incorporation of the virus in the tissue was assured by keeping the needle in place for 10 min after the end of perfusion. The head was sutured and mice were kept under observation until recovery from the anesthesia, before returning to the animal facility. In order to ensure a good expression of ChR2, electrophysiological experiments were performed 21–28 days after viral injection.

ChR2 Expression in Acute Cerebellar Slices

In five mice that were injected with the construct but were not used for in vivo recordings, after 30 days the cerebellum was removed and used to prepare acute slices (220 μm thick) following standard procedures (Mapelli et al., 2017). The efficacy of ChR2 expression was tested by extracellular recordings from PCs. The PC soma was selectively illuminated with blue led light (Polygon400, Mightex Systems). The extracellular signals were recorded using a Multiclamp 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices) controlled by pClamp10 through a Digidata1440A (Molecular Devices). When illuminated, the PCs increased firing activity as expected from effective ChR2 expression causing membrane depolarization (Supplementary Figure S1).

General Aspects of Optogenetics Experiments

Since our aim was to interfere with PC activity, the site of AAV1 injection was limited to restricted regions of the molecular layer (Supplementary Figure S1A). The ChR2 was expressed under a generic neuronal promoter in common to molecular layer interneurons, PCs and parallel fibers. Since local circuit wiring in the molecular layer is not homogeneous (Valera et al., 2016), optogenetic activation was not expected to sort out the same effect in all cases (cfr. Figure 1A for a scheme of the circuit). Indeed, depending on the individual experiment, optogenetic stimulation could either increase or decrease PC activity and the pause and, in 2 out of 8 cases, no response modification was detected.

Light Application During in vivo Recordings

A light-conducting glass fiber with 120 μm diameter cladding and numerical aperture NA = 0.22 (Thomas Recording GmBh, Germany) was mounted in the Eckhorn Matrix (Thomas Recording GmBh, Germany). Just as the recording electrodes, it was possible to drive the tip of the glass fiber down into the tissue with micrometric precision. The optic fiber was connected though a FC/PC patch cable (ThorLabs Ø105 μm, 0.22NA, FC/PC-FC/PC Fiber Patch Cable, 1 m) to a 473 nm MM laser (S1FC473MM fiber coupled laser, Thorlabs) with adjustable output power (50 mW maximum). The laser was gated by a (TTL) trigger signal generated by the RZ5D bioamp processor (Tucker Davis Technologies, Alachua, FL, United States) driven by the OpenEx software controlling data acquisition. YFP fluorescence allowed to determine the effectiveness of adenoviral expression in vivo. The tip of the fiber was placed at about 250 μm from the surface of the cerebellum, in order to obtain a localized optical stimulation of the molecular and PC layers. Laser light pulses (50 ms) were applied at 0.5 Hz paired to the air-puff (delay of 30 ms) with a power of 0.5–1 mW. The output power, measured with a power meter (PM100D, with s130c sensor; Thorlabs) at the tip of the glass fiber, was 0.03 mW (Kruse et al., 2014).

Histology

The location of recording electrodes in the DCN was confirmed histologically. Electrical lesions were obtained at the end of recordings by applying a 20 μA–20 s current pulse through the same recording electrode connected to a stimulus isolator and a stimulator unit. Then, the mouse was perfused transcardially with phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (Sigma-Aldrich) overnight at 4°C. The fixated brains were cryo-protected with 30% sucrose solution in PBS, embedded in OCT (Cryostat embedding medium, Killik, Bio-Optica), and stored at -80°C. 20-μm-thick histological sections were obtained and stained with toluidine blue. The histological confirmation of the recording sites was obtained by microscopic observation of the stained sections (see Figure 1B).

The identification of the viral expression was also analyzed histologically from the fixated brain of injected mice. Confocal images (see Supplementary Figure S1) were taken from 20-μm-thick sections washed with PBS (three times for 5 min), counterstained with Hoechst 33258 (Thermo Fisher Scientific- 2 μg/mL) for 15 min, washed again with PBS (three times for 5 min) and finally mounted with ProLongTM Gold Antifade Mountant (Thermo Fisher Scientific). Images were acquired with a TCS SP5 II (Leica Microsystems) equipped with a DM IRBE inverted microscope (Leica Microsystems) with 20, 40, or 63X objectives and visualized by LAS-AF Lite software (Leica Microsystems Application Suite Advanced Fluorescence Lite version 2.6.0) or with ImageJ (Fiji distribution, SciJava). Fluorescence microscopy showed that the site of injection was confined to limited regions of the molecular layer (see Supplementary Figure S1).

Data Analysis and Statistics

Electrophysiological signals were acquired using OpenEx software (Tucker-Davis Technologies) and analyzed offline using custom-written routines in MATLAB (Mathworks, Natick, MA, United States) and Excel. Openscope (part of the OpenEx suite) was used to construct online PSTH triggered by air-puffs, in order to identify responding units. The raw traces were analyzed and sorted offline using SpikeTrain (Neurasmus BV, Rotterdam, Netherlands) running under MATLAB. The stability of recordings was carefully assessed ( < ± 20% amplitude fluctuation over the duration of the recording) and only units with stable spike size were considered for further analysis. PSTHs and raster plots were used for the analysis of responses to stimulation, normally consisting of peaks and pauses emerging from background discharge. To optimize PSTH resolution, a 5 ms bin width was used to analyze peaks and a 15 ms bin width was used to analyze pauses. The “burst” was defined as an increase in firing frequency generating a PSTH peak after the stimulus. The “pause” was defined as a decrease in firing frequency generating a PSTH pause after the stimulus. The threshold for peaks and pauses detection in PSTHs was set at twice the standard deviation of the basal frequency in the pre-stimulus period, calculated for each bin. No constraints on the number of bins showing significant changes compared to the pre-stimulus period were applied, since some response might show small duration (as the case of peaks, lasting 5–10 ms and therefore described by one or few bins). Statistical comparisons of peak and pause changes in optogenetic experiments was performed against changes in the stability controls at the same experimental times (histograms in Figures 3B, 4F).

The effect of TSS was evaluated by measuring the corresponding changes in PSTH peaks and pauses as the post-TSS responses (computed over the first 15 min after TSS) that exceeded twice the standard deviation of the pre-TSS response (computed over the last 15 min before TSS). Positive changes were considered as a potentiation and negative changes were considered as a suppression of basal firing (a minority of units did not show any significant changes with respect to this criterion).

Statistical comparisons were carried out using paired or unpaired Student’s t-test or Fisher’s F-test. The normality of the data was assessed using the Shapiro–Wilk test. In the few cases data were not distributed normally, the Brown-Forsythe test was applied to assess the homogeneity of variances. Data in the text are reported as mean ± SEM. Clustering k-mean analysis and autocorrelation analysis on PSTHs were performed using MATLAB routines. Autocorrelations were performed using a function (xcorr) yielding oscillation frequency and magnitude (with magnitudes normalized to 1). The statistical significance of the changes in oscillation frequency in pharmacological and optogenetic experiments was evaluated with respect to stability controls at the same experimental times.

Data fitting was performed using routines written in OriginPro8 (OriginLab, Co., Northampton, MA, United States). A Lorentzian function was used to fit the frequency-dependence of plasticity changes:

y=y0+2Aπw4(xfc)2+w2

where y0 and A are curve baseline and amplitude, w is curve width, fc is the resonance frequency.

Results

Single-unit recordings were performed from the cerebellar fastigial nucleus in urethane anesthetized mice (Figure 1A). All units were spontaneously active and showed a basal frequency of 8.19 ± 0.99 Hz (range: 2–27 Hz; n = 51), in agreement with previous reports of spontaneous activity under urethane anesthesia (Sweeney et al., 1992; LeDoux et al., 1998; Raman et al., 2000). The recording site was confirmed by electric lesions made through the recording electrode and identified histologically (Figure 1B). Single-unit responses to low frequency tactile stimulation (0.5 Hz) generated spike bursts and pauses modifying the basal discharge (Figure 1C) that were likely to reflect the neuronal response to excitatory and inhibitory synaptic inputs impinging onto DCN neurons (Rowland and Jaeger, 2005, 2008).

Bursts and Pauses in DCN Unit Responses

Single-unit responses to low frequency tactile stimulation generated combinations of peaks and pauses in PSTHs and, in some cases, the response continued with an oscillation (see below). Over a total of 51 units, we identified 2 fundamental categories of patterns, with either the burst or the pause as the initial response (Figure 2A).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Bursts and pauses in DCN unit responses to tactile stimulation. (A) Example of PSTHs obtained from DCN units showing different responses to tactile stimulation (arrows): double burst (5 ms-bin), burst-pause (5 ms-bin), and pause-burst (15 ms-bin). Red dashed lines show the basal discharge frequency. The scale bars in the inset on top are 5 sp/bin and 25 ms. (B) In the pool of responses starting with a burst, two groups were discriminated using cluster analysis (k-means) on peak latency and burst duration. This results in the identification of early and late peaks, whose latencies are compatible with inputs from the trigeminal and cortical pathways conveying sensory stimuli to the cerebellum (cfr. Figure 1A). (C) Characterization of pause-burst responses. A positive correlation was found between rebound-peak amplitude and pause depth [R2 = 0.87, Fisher’s F-test p(F) = 0.001, n = 8].

When the burst initiated the response (“burst-first” category, n = 26), some units (n = 18) showed a single PSTH peak with latency of 14.27 ± 4.07 ms (duration 11.80 ± 2.51 ms), while others (n = 8) showed two PSTH peaks with latencies of 9.87 ± 1.99 and 33 ± 3.7 ms (duration of 8.12 ± 1.31 and 15 ± 3.27 ms). These peak latencies corresponded to those reported for trigeminal and cortical responses of granular layer neurons (Vos et al., 1999; Roggeri et al., 2008), suggesting that the initial bursts most likely corresponded to synaptic excitation of DCN neurons through trigeminal and cortical mossy fibers (Figure 2B; see section “Materials and Methods” for details).

In a subset of experiments, selective AMPA and NMDA receptor antagonists (100 μM NBQX + 250 μM D-APV, respectively) were injected in the fastigial nucleus close to the recording site. In 4 (out of 4) neurons that initiated the response with a burst, the burst was abolished (peak change -95.9 ± 12.8%, n = 4; paired Student’s t-test; p = 0.026), while pauses remained unaltered (pause depth change -16.2 ± 10.2%; n = 4, paired Student’s t-test; p = 0.14; see below) (Figure 3A).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Pharmacological and optogenetic manipulations of burst and pause responses. (A) Example of PSTHs from a DCN unit showing a burst as initial response (left), that was prevented (right) by excitatory transmission blockers (NBQX and D-APV) injection in the nucleus (red dashed lines show the basal discharge frequency). The gray dashed rectangles show the areas that are overlapped in the inset. The histogram shows the % of the response, whether PSTH peaks or pauses, left after blockers injection, compared to control (n = 4 for both; paired Student’s t-test; p < 0.05). (B) Example of PSTHs from a DCN unit showing burst-pause response before (left) and during (right) optogenetic stimulation of the molecular layer (the blue rectangle showing the time and duration of laser activation; red dashed lines show the basal discharge frequency). The histograms on the right show the percent change on the peak amplitude of the excitatory response and on the pause area (obtained by multiplying pause depth and duration) during optogenetic stimulation compared to control, in the single units recorded. The gray shadows show the average % change observed at the same time points in the stability controls (see section “Materials and Methods”). Note that peak amplitude is not affected, while the pause is significantly modified.

When the pause initiated the response (“pause-first” category, n = 25), it occurred with a latency of 28.8 ± 5.2 ms (duration 25.0 ± 3.8 ms). This delay was compatible with signal transmission along the mossy fiber – granule cell – PC – DCN neuronal pathway (Ramakrishnan et al., 2016), rather than resulting from local interneurons, suggesting that the initial pause most likely corresponded to DCN neuron inhibition by PCs. In a subset of experiments (n = 8), optogenetic stimulation of the molecular layer was applied to disrupt the cortical output, by delivering a light impulse 30 ms after the air-puff, i.e., in coincidence with the pause. In six of these recordings optogenetic stimulation caused a change in pause depth and duration exceeding three times the standard deviation of time-matched controls (see section “Materials and Methods” and histograms in Figure 3B). It should be noted that the pause in three cases increased and in three cases decreased, possibly reflecting the balance between optogenetic activation of PCs and molecular layer interneurons (see section “Materials and Methods” and Figure 1A and Supplementary Figure S1 for details). In the remaining 2 units, no evident effect of optogenetics was observed.

Several units initiating the response with a burst (21 out of 26) showed a pause following peak(s), and some units initiating the response with a pause (8 out of 24) showed a burst following the pause (Figure 2A). The nature of these burst-pause and pause-burst patterns showed peculiar properties.

In burst-pause responses, the pause was significantly delayed (58.17 ± 5.52 ms, n = 21, unpaired Student’s t-test; p = 0.00039) compared to that measured when it initiated the response. This longer delay suggests the intervention of additional mechanisms, like signal reentry through the recently discovered DCN – granule cells connections (Gao et al., 2016) or through precerebellar nuclei (Kistler and De Zeeuw, 2003), capable of protracting and enhancing PCs activation through cerebello-cortical loops. Phase reset, an intrinsic electroresponsive phenomenon observed in neurons (e.g., see Solinas et al., 2007), was unlikely to be responsible for this effect, as explained below (see Figure 4D).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Stimulus-induced oscillations in DCN units. (A) PSTH obtained from a DCN unit showing low frequency oscillation following a burst-pause response. (B) Autocorrelogram obtained from the unit shown in (A) (oscillation frequency 3.15 Hz; magnitude 0.16). (C) The magnitude and frequency of oscillations deriving from the autocorrelation analysis shown in (B) were plotted for each unit. The k-means clustering revealed two groups of data, characterized by high frequency – low magnitude oscillations (black symbols) and low frequency – high magnitude oscillations (green and red symbols, for pause-first and burst-first responses respectively). The units in which the TSS was delivered are represented as circles, while those in which the TSS was not delivered are represented as squares. Gray filled symbols are used for the units in which pharmacology was applied, while blue-filled circles are used for the units in the optogenetics experiments. The arrow indicates the unit shown in (A,B). (D) Relationship between stimulus-induced oscillation frequency and spontaneous firing frequency for the low frequency – high magnitude oscillation units in (C). The linear fitting shows no evident trend [R2 = 0.03, Fisher’s F-test p(F) = 0.89]. (E) Relationship between magnitude of stimulus-induced oscillations and spontaneous firing frequency for the low frequency – high magnitude oscillation units in (C). The linear fitting suggests a positive trend [R2 = 0.25, Fisher’s F-test p(F) < 0.08]. In (C–E), the data points are divided into burst-first and pause-first units, and circles represent the units that were further used for plasticity induction (see Figures 5, 6). (F) The histogram shows the percent change in stimulus-induced oscillation frequency during optogenetics in the same units reported in (C–E). The gray shadow shows the average percent change observed at the same time points in the stability controls (see section “Materials and Methods”).

In pause-burst responses, the bursts followed with a latency of 90.61 ± 20.63 ms (duration 12.14 ± 3.54 ms, unpaired Student’s t-test; p = 0.01), that was significantly longer compared to that measured when it initiated the response. A positive correlation was found between pause depth and the subsequent peak amplitude [R2 = 0.89, Fisher’s F-test p(F) < 0.001, n = 8] (Figure 2C). A plausible explanation is that these bursts are non-synaptic and reflect post-inhibitory rebound discharge in DCN neurons (Alviña et al., 2008; Witter et al., 2013; Canto et al., 2016), which is the stronger the deeper the pause. This is supported by a recording in which the AMPA and NMDA receptor antagonists were injected in the fastigial nucleus while recording a pause-burst unit. In this case, the burst following the pause was unaffected (single observation, not shown).

Spontaneous Activity and Stimulus-Induced Oscillations in DCN Units

The PSTH elicited by tactile stimulation in several cases showed an oscillation following the initial peaks and pauses (Figure 4A). This oscillatory pattern was apparent in autocorrelation analysis (Figure 4B). The frequency/magnitude plot revealed a negative trend, with slower oscillations showing larger magnitude and vice versa (Figure 4C). K-means analysis identified two significantly different clusters of points (unpaired Student’s t-test; p = 0.00175), one at higher and the other at lower frequency. Low-frequency oscillations averaged 12.7 ± 1.0 Hz, n = 34.

The relationship between low-frequency stimulus-induced oscillations and spontaneous activity is shown in Figures 4D,E. No significant correlation was found either for frequency [R2 = 0.03, Fisher’s F-test p(F) = 0.89, n = 34] or magnitude [R2 = 0.25, Fisher’s F-test p(F) < 0.08, n = 34]. It should be noted that, out of 34 units, 15 were of the burst-first and 19 of the pause-first category. At a closer analysis, the burst-first units showed a significantly higher magnitude (0.11 ± 0.01 vs. 0.08 ± 0.01; unpaired Student’s t-test; p = 0.037) and lower spontaneous frequency (6.2 ± 0.8 vs. 10.0 ± 1.1 Hz, unpaired Student’s t-test; p = 0.01) than the pause-first units (Figures 4C–E) suggesting the existence of two distinct functional classes of DCN neurons (see below).

The injection of AMPA and NMDA receptor antagonists in the fastigial nucleus did not modify the stimulus induced oscillation frequency of the units (average absolute variation from control of 5.1 ± 0.6%, not different from that of stability controls of 5.1 ± 0.5%, n = 4 and n = 10 respectively; unpaired Student’s t-test; p = 0.99). Conversely, optogenetic stimulation of the molecular layer caused a change in the stimulus induced oscillation frequency of the recorded units exceeding three times the standard deviation of time-matched controls (see section “Materials and Methods” and Figure 4F). It should be noted that the induced-oscillation frequency in two cases increased and in four cases decreased, possibly reflecting the balance between optogenetic activation of PCs and molecular layer interneurons.

Long-Lasting Changes Induced by TSS in DCN Unit Responses

The cerebellar cortex in rodents is known to respond to TSS of the whisker pad with long-lasting changes in the granular and molecular layers (Roggeri et al., 2008; Diwakar et al., 2011; Prestori et al., 2013; Ramakrishnan et al., 2016). We therefore investigated whether the delivery of the same TSS pattern was able to affect DCN neuron responsiveness. We defined Spike-Related Potentiation (SR-P) and Spike-Related Suppression (SR-S) as the increase or decrease in spike response probability with respect to baseline (cf. Ramakrishnan et al., 2016), both in bursts and pauses. For simplicity, we considered only the initial bursts and pauses, since their amplitude is not influenced by preceding electrical events. The values of changes were measured for each unit in the first 15 min following TSS with respect to the last 15 min before TSS.

TSS-Induced Changes in Initial Bursts

Theta-sensory stimulation was delivered in 13 recordings showing an initial excitatory burst (burst-first, Figure 5A). A significant SR-P of the first PSTH peak was observed in four units (45.27 ± 10.73%, n = 4, paired Student’s t-test; p = 0.01; Figure 5A), while a significant SR-S was observed in another 8 units (-27.16 ± 5.34%, paired Student’s t-test; p = 0.01). Only in 1 out of these 13 units, no significant changes were observed.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Long-lasting changes induced by TSS in DCN unit responses. (A) Example of PSTHs illustrating the peak changes (SR-P or SR-S) induced by TSS in DCN units of the burst-first category. The histogram shows the average percent changes in PSTH peak amplitude for all the units showing SR-P, SR-S, or stability controls (no TSS; paired Student’s t-test; p < 0.05, ∗∗p < 0.01). (B) Example of PSTHs illustrating the pause changes (SR-P or SR-S) induced by TSS in DCN units of the pause-first category. The histogram shows the average percent changes in PSTH peak amplitude for all the units showing SR-P, SR-S, or stability controls (no TSS; paired Student’s t-test; ∗∗p < 0.01). (C) Average time-course of peak (in burst-first units, left) and pause (in pause-first units, right) amplitude percent changes normalized to the control period before TSS (dashed line) in the units showing SR-P, SR-S and in the stability group (TSS not delivered). Note that the response changes for peaks and pauses differed significantly from the stability controls in the first 15 min after the TSS.

TSS-Induced Changes in Initial Pauses

Theta-sensory stimulation was delivered in 13 recordings showing an initial pause in the response (pause-first, Figure 5B). A significant SR-P of the pause was observed in 4 units (48.43 ± 14.77%, n = 4, paired Student’s t-test; p = 0.004), while a significant SR-S of the pause depth was observed in another 7 units (-27.19 ± 7.65%, n = 7, paired Student’s t-test; p = 0.007; Figure 5B). In 2 units, no pause depth changes were found.

Stability Controls

In 7 units showing an initial excitatory burst and in 5 units showing an initial pause, TSS was not delivered. In these units, the bursts and pauses remained stable for a duration similar to that of experiments in which the TSS was delivered (bursts: 4.7 ± 11.7% change, paired Student’s t-test, n = 7, p = 0.9; pauses: 5.6 ± 10.0% change, n = 5, paired Student’s t-test; p = 0.1). These controls ruled out possible spurious changes due to intrinsic response amplitude fluctuations with time.

Average Time Course

The average time course for burst and pause changes was constructed by grouping all the units in each given category. The SR-P and SR-S of peaks and pauses reported above, which were statistically significant for the first 15 min after TSS, returned back to baseline within 30 min (Figure 5C).

Correlation Between Long-Lasting Changes and Stimulus-Induced Oscillations

In Figure 4, two functional classes of units have been identified based on their response pattern and low frequency oscillatory properties, summing up to a total of 15 burst-first units and 17 pause-first units. Here we have considered the relationship between long-lasting changes and stimulus-induced oscillation frequency in the 8 burst-first and 10 pause-first units that received TSS in control condition (without pharmacological manipulation or optogenetics). The changes were weakly correlated to frequency, with larger changes occurring at lower frequencies [linear correlation: R2 = -0.07 and p(F) < 0.05 for burst-first units; R2 = 0.32 p(F) < 0.03 for pause-first units]. The hypothesis that changes were centered on the theta-band was assessed by fitting the data using resonant functions. In particular, the Lorentzian distribution fitted the data better than linear, suggesting that peak and pause changes after TSS might be correlated with the frequency of stimulus-induced oscillations. By using a Lorentzian distribution, peak changes in the burst-first units (Figure 6A) peaked at 9.2 Hz with 53% SR-P and settled down to -17.2% SR-S at lower and higher frequencies [R2 = 0.83; Fisher’s F-test p(F) < 0.01]. Pause changes in the pause-first units (Figure 6B) peaked at 5.5 Hz with -42% SR-S and settled to 26.2% SR-P at higher frequencies [R2 = 0.78; Fisher’s F-test p(F) < 0.02]. Thus, Lorentzian fitting of SR-P and SR-S distributions showed opposite changes in burst-first and pause-first units with peaks in the low frequency range of stimulus-induced oscillations.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Frequency-dependence of long-lasting changes after TSS. (A) The plot shows the distribution of peak amplitude changes after TSS in burst-first units with respect to stimulus induced oscillation frequency. The Lorentzian fitting [R2 = 0.83; Fisher’s F-test p(F) = 0.01] shows a peak at 9.2 Hz. (B) The plot shows the distribution of pause amplitude changes after TSS in pause-first units with respect to stimulus induced oscillation frequency. The Lorentzian fitting [R2 = 0.78; Fisher’s F-test p(F) = 0.02] shows a peak at 5.5 Hz. Both in (A,B), open symbols identify the same low-frequency units reported in Figure 4 and filled symbols are the average values ( ± SEM) of high frequency oscillation units. Both in (A,B), the gray area shows the 95% confidence interval of the fitting.

We then asked whether and how these long-lasting changes were influenced by cerebellar cortical activity. To this end, we used optogenetic stimulation of the cerebellar molecular layer, which allows a broader activation than electrical stimulation and is therefore more likely to capture neuronal chains involved in controlling the recorded DCN units. We have shown above that optogenetic stimulation of the molecular layer could indeed modify DCN responsiveness (see Figures 3B, 4F), disrupting the cortical output by modifying PC firing. It should be noted that, as explained in Section “Materials and Methods,” this test was not expected to yield a deterministic increase or decrease in PC firing, but rather to impact on DCN units and change their ability to generate long-term response changes after TSS. We thus compared DCN units response changes with or without the use of optogenetics assuming the Lorentzian distribution as the best fit to our data. Optogenetic stimulation of the molecular layer during TSS altered the long-lasting changes compared to those expected from controls, in such a way that these always fell beyond the confidence limits predicted from control data, both for peaks in burst-first units (Figure 7A) and for pauses in pause-first units (Figure 7B). The distance from the control curves in Figures 6A,B, estimated at the frequency of stimulus-induced oscillation recorded during optogenetic stimulation, was 45.1 ± 7.0% (n = 6, unpaired Student’s t-test; p = 0.03; significantly different from the distance from the same curve calculated from control data: 9.1 ± 2.1, n = 20, unpaired Student’s t-test; p = 0.0028; Figure 7C). Therefore, optogenetics did not seem to primarily address the same mechanism of frequency-dependent induction of long-lasting changes occurring in DCN units but rather to affect different mechanisms, presumably located in the molecular layer (see section “Discussion”).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. The impact of optogenetic stimulation on long-lasting response changes. (A) The plot shows the Lorentzian fitting as Figure 6A, with the gray area showing the 95% confidence interval. Note that the data-points representing burst-first units in which the TSS was paired with optogenetics fall far outside the confidence interval. (B) The plot shows the Lorentzian fitting as in Figure 6B, with the gray area showing the 95% confidence interval, extrapolated beyond the last point of control to compare new data. Note that the data-points representing pause-first units in which the TSS was paired with optogenetics fall far outside the confidence interval (except for the two points representing the units in which optogenetics did not show any effect; crossed circles). (C) The histogram shows the average distance of the optogenetics data points from the fitting curves in (A,B). The gray shadow shows the average distance of control data in Figure 6 from the same fitting curves.

Discussion

Deep cerebellar nuclei units were spontaneously active and responded to tactile sensory stimulation with different combinations of bursts, pauses and oscillations. Following theta-frequency stimulation (TSS), DCN units showed spike-related potentiation or suppression, SR-P or SR-S, both in bursts and pauses. To our knowledge, SR-P and SR-S are the first electrophysiological evidence that long-lasting changes can be observed following naturally patterned sensory entrainment in DCN neurons in vivo. Unique in the cerebellum among the other long-lasting changes observed in vivo (Roggeri et al., 2008; Gao et al., 2012; D’Angelo, 2014; D’Angelo et al., 2016a; Ramakrishnan et al., 2016), the DCN SR-P/SR-S distributions were correlated to the stimulus-induced oscillation frequency of DCN units through Lorentzian functions peaking in the theta-frequency range, disclosing the complex nature of the underlying plasticity mechanisms.

The Nature of DCN Unit Responses

All DCN units responded to tactile stimulation with short delays typical of the fast cerebellar reaction to sensory inputs. In some units (53%), bursts were the first DCN response and occurred either as a single peak at 10–14 ms or a double peak (in a third of cases) about 10 ms later. This pattern closely matches that observed in the granular layer (Morissette and Bower, 1996; Vos et al., 1999; Roggeri et al., 2008), suggesting that DCN neurons can receive double mossy fiber activation through the trigeminal pathway and the somato-sensory cortex [the trigeminal connection might not be direct for the fastigial nucleus though (Morcuende et al., 2002; Rowland and Jaeger, 2005, 2008)]. The excitatory nature of these bursts was confirmed by their extinction after injection of AMPA and NMDA receptors blockers into the DCN. In the remaining units (47%), pauses were the first DCN response with delays of 25–29 ms, most likely reflecting signal transfer through the cerebellar cortex down to PCs and DCN. This delay can be accounted for by considering that PC excitation through mossy fibers and granule cells takes about 15 ms (Ramakrishnan et al., 2016) and an additional time is required to inhibit DCN cells. Indeed, optogenetic stimulation of the molecular layer was able to modulate pause duration and depth.

It should be noted that, in principle, climbing fibers could also contribute to DCN excitation through axonal collaterals. However, in comparable recording conditions (Ramakrishnan et al., 2016), PC complex spikes that reflect climbing fiber activation were only sporadically observed and had a latency of 40–50 ms, which is too long to explain the latency of the DCN unit responses. Although climbing fibers could contribute to DCN activation, when actively stimulated (Mogensen et al., 2017), it seems very unlikely that they took part to generate the PSTH peaks analyzed here.

The pauses occurring after initial bursts were also modified by optogenetic stimulation of the molecular layer and reflected therefore DCN inhibition by PCs. These pauses, which were evident about 50 ms after the stimulus, could have been protracted by signal reentry into the cerebellar cortex through intracerebellar (Ankri et al., 2015; Gao et al., 2016) or extracerebellar loops (Kistler and De Zeeuw, 2003). The bursts following the pauses correlated with the depth of the preceding pause and were therefore probably rebound activities due to intrinsic electroresponsiveness (Alviña et al., 2008; Hoebeek et al., 2010; Witter et al., 2013) (this conclusion was supported by rebound burst persistence after injection of AMPA and NMDA receptors blockers into the DCN in a single experiment).

Some units continued their response with an oscillatory cycle independent on whether the responses started with a burst or a pause. The frequency and magnitude of oscillations induced by stimulation were not significantly correlated with spontaneous discharge in the same units. Therefore, oscillations could not be explained by phase reset, since in that case the two frequencies should coincide (Solinas et al., 2007). The origin of these oscillations should then reflect circuit mechanisms (see Figure 1A). A first mechanism was hypothesized by Yarom (Jacobson et al., 2008; Chen et al., 2010) and involves DCN control of inferior olive (IO) oscillations that reverberate through climbing fibers into the DCN-PC-IO loop. Stimulus-induced oscillations similar to ours are indeed evident in the PSTH of DCN neurons following direct climbing fiber activation (Cheron and Cheron, 2018). A second mechanism hypothesized by De Zeeuw could involve signal reentry through extra-cerebellar circuits (Kistler and De Zeeuw, 2003; Gao et al., 2016) or through the more recently identified connections between DCN and granular layer (Ankri et al., 2015; Gao et al., 2016), therefore passing again through PCs. That PCs could actually be a node in the loops controlling the stimulus-induced oscillations was supported by their perturbation by optogenetic stimulation of the molecular layer.

There were two groups of units showing stimulus-induced ocillations, burst-first and pause-first units, which turned out to show opposite frequency-dependent changes following TSS. The potential relationship between these functional groups and the DCN neuron subpopulations reported in vitro (Bagnall et al., 2009; Uusisaari and Knopfel, 2012) remains to be determined.

Long-Lasting Changes in DCN Unit Responses and Their Relationship With Plastic Mechanisms

By being connected to the sensory input through multi-synaptic chains, the long-lasting changes in DCN unit responses could either be generated locally or occur upstream in the cerebellar cortex.

On one hand, according to fittings using Lorentzian functions, SR-P peaked at 9.2 Hz in burst-first units and SR-S at 5.5 Hz in pause-first units. This property favors the engagement of local mechanisms, since no similar frequency-dependent changes have ever been observed either in the granular or molecular layer in vivo (Roggeri et al., 2008; Ramakrishnan et al., 2016) or in vitro (for review see Hansel et al., 2001; Gao et al., 2012; D’Angelo, 2014; D’Angelo et al., 2016b). Moreover, LTP and LTD reported in DCN neurons in vitro are based on specific sequences of excitation, inhibition and rebounds (Morishita and Sastry, 1996; Ouardouz and Sastry, 2000; Pugh and Raman, 2006; Zhang and Linden, 2006). Therefore, the different synapses impinging on DCN neurons could reciprocally influence one each other, providing a plausible mechanism for SR-P and SR-S induction during stimulus-induced oscillations. The robust potentiation in PC responses observed in vivo following TSS (Ramakrishnan et al., 2016) is likely to contribute to DCN plasticity in pause-first units, independently of the frequency of stimulus-induced oscillations.

On the other hand, SR-P and SR-S of the initial burst and pause were remarkably altered by optogenetic stimulation of the molecular layer. Optogenetic stimulation caused long-lasting changes in DCN unit responses that went much beyond those expected from the alteration of stimulus-dependent oscillation frequency. This suggests the engagement of additional mechanisms. For example, similar to electrical stimulation, optogenetic stimulation might cause a broad set of long-lasting changes in the molecular layer (for details see, Ramakrishnan et al., 2016) modifying the PC output, which would eventually perturb the long-lasting changes observed in DCN unit responses. Further insights on the role of cortical input on DCN responses to TSS might derive from the use of genetically modified models with known alterations in the cerebellar cortex (e.g., mice lacking the phosphatase PP2B in PCs, that show selective loss of PC potentiation, as in Schonewille et al., 2010; Romano et al., 2018).

A further issue is about the time course of SR-P and SR-S, which decayed over 30 min. Both in rodents and humans, cerebellar learning has been predicted to occur in two steps, a faster one in the cerebellar cortex and a slower one in DCN (Medina and Mauk, 2000; Attwell et al., 2001; Smith et al., 2006; Monaco et al., 2014). Mathematical modeling further predicts that fast plasticity at the parallel fiber-PC synapse would be able to tune slow and more stable plasticity in DCN (Medina et al., 2001; Garrido et al., 2013; Casellato et al., 2014). So why in vivo recordings have shown more persistent changes in the granular layer (Roggeri et al., 2008) and molecular layer (Ramakrishnan et al., 2016) than in DCN? There are three key issues to consider. First, more intense or repeated stimulation may be needed to promote plasticity consolidation in DCN. Secondly, here DCN was not entrained in active sensorimotor feedback that can enhance cerebellar oscillations (Marshall and Lang, 2004). Thirdly, there was no attentional or motivational state, as the animal was anesthetized. Indeed, neuromodulation by noradrenaline, acetylcholine or serotonin is thought to be critical to drive oscillations and plasticity and promote learning (Sugihara et al., 1995; Schweighofer et al., 2004). It should also be noted that a similar trend, with a stimulus inducing long-term plasticity in the cerebellar cortex but having less effects on the cerebellar nuclei, has been recently described for the anterior interposed nucleus in cats (Mogensen et al., 2017).

It cannot be excluded that stimulus-induced oscillations and the induction or expression of long-lasting changes in DCN neuron responses might have been influenced by anesthesia. We notice, however, that urethane is very conservative on the NMDA and GABA-A receptor-dependent mechanisms of neurotransmission and has been successfully used to demonstrate long-lasting changes at other cerebellar synapses in vivo (Roggeri et al., 2008; Ramakrishnan et al., 2016). Moreover, stimulus-induced oscillations in DCN neurons have been recently shown using ketamine-xylidodihydrothiazin anesthesia (Cheron and Cheron, 2018).

Conclusion

The identification of a possible relationship between DCN long-lasting changes and oscillatory dynamics engaged by tactile stimuli suggests that two key cerebellar functions can be reconciled: oscillatory activity in DCN may not just be needed to gate motor activity (Llinas, 1988; Marshall and Lang, 2004) but also to control plasticity and acquisition of sensorimotor engrams (D’Angelo and De Zeeuw, 2009; Cheron et al., 2016). Interestingly, the preferential frequency of long-lasting DCN response changes identified by Lorentzian fittings was in the theta-band, i.e., the characteristic oscillatory frequency of the IO-PC-DCN circuit (Jacobson et al., 2008) and of the cerebello-extracerebellar loops (Kistler and De Zeeuw, 2003) following mossy fiber inputs. It is tempting to speculate that the cerebellum uses oscillating and resonant mechanisms similar to those that are known to favor the induction of plasticity in hippocampal and cortical synapses (Buzsaki, 2006; Roy et al., 2014) and that the frequency of oscillations provides a signal binding DCN plasticity to specific neuronal ensembles and brain states (Buzsaki, 2005; Buzsaki, 2006; Timofeev, 2011). This would be eventually reflected into neuro-muscular coherence on the systemic scale (Gruart et al., 2000; Koekkoek et al., 2002; Sánchez-Campusano et al., 2007, 2009; Wang et al., 2018). The frequency dependence of burst and pause changes in DCN units in vivo prompts for a further characterization of LTP and LTD mechanisms in DCN neurons in vitro, also considering the existence of functionally distinct DCN neuronal populations. New experiments may also be conducted in awake animals and combined with computational modeling (Medina and Mauk, 2000; Casellato et al., 2014; Luque et al., 2014; D’Angelo et al., 2016a) to address the impact of frequency-dependent forms of plasticity during cerebellar adaptation and learning.

Data Availability

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Author Contributions

LeM performed in vivo recordings, data analysis, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. IM performed the sets of in vivo recordings with pharmacology and optogenetics, and data analysis. LDP performed the initial in vivo recordings. ST performed histology and image analysis. LiM and ED coordinated the work and wrote the manuscript. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

This project/research received funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 Framework Program for Research and Innovation under the Framework Partnership Agreement No. 650003 (HBP FPA); the European Union’s Horizon 2020 Framework Program for Research and Innovation under the Specific Grant Agreement No. 720270 (Human Brain Project SGA1), and under the Specific Grant Agreement No. 785907 (Human Brain Project SGA2). This work was supported by grant [7(17)] of Centro Fermi to ED, Blue-Sky Research grant of the University of Pavia (BSR77992) to LiM.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fncel.2019.00084/full#supplementary-material

FIGURE S1 | ChR2 expression in the cerebellar cortex of mice used for optogenetics. (A) Confocal image of green fluorescence (YFP) in the cerebellar cortex of a mouse sacrificed after in vivo recordings. Since YFP is expressed under the same promoter, YFP fluorescence reports the place of ChR2 expression. Note that fluorescence is mostly confined to the molecular and Purkinje cell layers. GL, granular layer; ML, molecular layer; PC, Purkinje cell. (B) Acute brain slice in bright field, with the recording electrode placed near a Purkinje cell (PC). The blue circle indicates that illuminated region is confined to PC soma and the trace shows the corresponding response in the stimulated PC (single trace, scale bars: 10 pA/200 ms). Note the increase in PC firing frequency during optical stimulation, followed by a pause configuring a typical burst-pause PC response (Cao et al., 2012; Herzfeld et al., 2015; Masoli and D’Angelo, 2017). GL, granular layer; ML, molecular layer.

References

Albus, J. (1971). The theory of cerebellar function. Math. Biosci. 10, 25–61. doi: 10.1016/0025-5564(71)90051-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alviña, K., Walter, J., Kohn, A., Ellis-Davies, G., and Khodakhah, K. (2008). Questioning the role of rebound firing in the cerebellum. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 1256–1258. doi: 10.1038/nn.2195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ankri, L., Husson, Z., Pietrajtis, K., Proville, R., Lena, C., Yarom, Y., et al. (2015). A novel inhibitory nucleo-cortical circuit controls cerebellar Golgi cell activity. Elife 4:e06262. doi: 10.7554/eLife.06262

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antonietti, A., Casellato, C., Garrido, J. A., Luque, N. R., Naveros, F., Ros, E. E. D. A., et al. (2016). Spiking neural network with distributed plasticity reproduces cerebellar learning in eye blink conditioning paradigms. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 63, 210–219. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2015.2485301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Attwell, P. J. E., Rahman, S., and Yeo, C. H. (2001). Acquisition of eyeblink conditioning is critically dependent on normal function in cerebellar cortical lobule HVI. J. Neurosci. 21, 5715–5722. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-15-05715.2001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bagnall, M. W., Zingg, B., Sakatos, A., Moghadam, S. H., Zeilhofer, H. U., and du Lac, S. (2009). Glycinergic projection neurons of the cerebellum. J. Neurosci. 29, 10104–10110. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2087-09.2009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bengtsson, F., and Jorntell, H. (2007). Ketamine and xylazine depress sensory-evoked parallel fiber and climbing fiber responses. J. Neurophysiol. 98, 1697–1705. doi: 10.1152/jn.00057.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bower, J. M., and Woolston, D. C. (1983). Congruence of spatial organization of tactile projections to granule cell and Purkinje cell layers of cerebellar hemispheres of the albino rat: vertical organization of cerebellar cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 49, 745–766. doi: 10.1152/jn.1983.49.3.745

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsaki, G. (2005). Theta rhythm of navigation: link between path integration and landmark navigation, episodic and semantic memory. Hippocampus 15, 827–840. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsaki, G. (2006). Rhythms of the Brain. New York, NY: Oxford University Press US. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195301069.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Canto, C. B., Witter, L., and De Zeeuw, C. I. (2016). Whole-cell properties of cerebellar nuclei neurons in vivo. PLoS One 11:e0165887. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0165887

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, Y., Maran, S. K., Dhamala, M., Jaeger, D., and Heck, D. H. (2012). Behavior-related pauses in simple-spike activity of mouse Purkinje cells are linked to spike rate modulation. J. Neurosci. 32, 8678–8685. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4969-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casellato, C., Antonietti, A., Garrido, J. A., Carrillo, R. R., Luque, N. R., Ros, E., et al. (2014). Adaptive robotic control driven by a versatile spiking cerebellar network. PLoS One 9:e112265. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0112265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casellato, C., Antonietti, A., Garrido, J. A., Carrillo, R. R., Luque, N. R., Ros, E., et al. (2015). Distributed cerebellar plasticity implements generalized multiple-scale memory components in real-robot sensorimotor tasks. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 9:24. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2015.00024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chadderton, P., Margrie, T. W., and Häusser, M. (2004). Integration of quanta in cerebellar granule cells during sensory processing. Nature 428, 856–860. doi: 10.1038/nature02442

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, X., Kovalchuk, Y., Adelsberger, H., Henning, H. A., Sausbier, M., Wietzorrek, G., et al. (2010). Disruption of the olivo-cerebellar circuit by Purkinje neuron-specific ablation of BK channels. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 12323–12328. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1001745107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheron, G., Marquez-Ruiz, J., and Dan, B. (2016). Oscillations, timing, plasticity, and learning in the cerebellum. Cerebellum 15, 122–138. doi: 10.1007/s12311-015-0665-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheron, J., and Cheron, G. (2018). Beta-gamma burst stimulations of the inferior olive induce high-frequency oscillations in the deep cerebellar nuclei. Eur. J. Neurosci. 48, 2879–2889. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13873

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Angelo, E. (2014). The organization of plasticity in the cerebellar cortex: from synapses to control. Prog. Brain Res. 210, 31–58. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-63356-9.00002-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Angelo, E., and De Zeeuw, C. I. (2009). Timing and plasticity in the cerebellum: focus on the granular layer. Trends Neurosci. 32, 30–40. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Angelo, E., Antonietti, A., Casali, S., Casellato, C., Garrido, J. A., Luque, N. R., et al. (2016a). Modeling the cerebellar microcircuit: new strategies for a long-standing issue. Front. Cell Neurosci. 10:176. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2016.00176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Angelo, E., Mapelli, L., Casellato, C., Garrido, J. A., Luque, N., Monaco, J., et al. (2016b). Distributed circuit plasticity: new clues for the cerebellar mechanisms of learning. Cerebellum 15, 139–151. doi: 10.1007/s12311-015-0711-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diwakar, S., Lombardo, P., Solinas, S., Naldi, G., and D’Angelo, E. (2011). Local field potential modeling predicts dense activation in cerebellar granule cells clusters under LTP and LTD control. PLoS One 6:e21928. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0021928

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekerot, C. F., and Jorntell, H. (2001). Parallel fibre receptive fields of Purkinje cells and interneurons are climbing fibre-specific. Eur. J. Neurosci. 13, 1303–1310. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01499.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, Z., Proietti-Onori, M., Lin, Z., Ten Brinke, M. M., Boele, H. J., Potters, J. W., et al. (2016). Excitatory cerebellar nucleocortical circuit provides internal amplification during associative conditioning. Neuron 89, 645–657. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, Z., van Beugen, B. J., and De Zeeuw, C. I. (2012). Distributed synergistic plasticity and cerebellar learning. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 619–635. doi: 10.1038/nrn3312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, Z., Davis, C., Thomas, A. M., Economo, M. N., Abrego, A. M., Svoboda, K., et al. (2018). A cortico-cerebellar loop for motor planning. Nature 563, 113–116. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0633-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrido, J. A., Luque, N. R., D’Angelo, E., and Ros, E. (2013). Distributed cerebellar plasticity implements adaptable gain control in a manipulation task: a closed-loop robotic simulation. Front. Neural Circuits 7:159. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Godaux, E., Cheron, G., and Mettens, P. (1990). Ketamine induces failure of the oculomotor neural integrator in the cat. Neurosci. Lett. 116, 162–167. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(90)90403-V

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gruart, A., Guillazo-Blanch, G., Fernandez-Mas, R., Jimenez-Diaz, L., and Delgado-Garcia, J. M. (2000). Cerebellar posterior interpositus nucleus as an enhancer of classically conditioned eyelid responses in alert cats. J. Neurophysiol. 84, 2680–2690. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.84.5.2680

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, A., Feng, J. Y., Li, J., Ding, N., Li, Y. J., Qiu, D. L., et al. (2016). Effects of norepinephrine on spontaneous firing activity of cerebellar Purkinje cells in vivo in mice. Neurosci. Lett. 629, 262–266. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2016.06.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hansel, C., Linden, D. J., and D’Angelo, E. (2001). Beyond parallel fiber LTD: the diversity of synaptic and non-synaptic plasticity in the cerebellum. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 467–475. doi: 10.1038/87419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hara, K., and Harris, R. A. (2002). The anesthetic mechanism of urethane: the effects on neurotransmitter-gated ion channels. Anesth. Analg. 94, 313–318; Table of contents.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Herzfeld, D. J., Kojima, Y., Soetedjo, R., and Shadmehr, R. (2015). Encoding of action by the Purkinje cells of the cerebellum. Nature 526, 439–442. doi: 10.1038/nature15693

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoebeek, F. E., Witter, L., Ruigrok, T. J., and De Zeeuw, C. I. (2010). Differential olivo-cerebellar cortical control of rebound activity in the cerebellar nuclei. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 8410–8415. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0907118107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ito, M. (1972). Neural design of the cerebellar motor control system. Brain Res. 40, 81–84. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(72)90110-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobson, G. A., Rokni, D., and Yarom, Y. (2008). A model of the olivo-cerebellar system as a temporal pattern generator. Trends Neurosci. 31, 617–625. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jahnsen, H. (1986a). Electrophysiological characteristics of neurones in the guinea-pig deep cerebellar nuclei in vitro. J. Physiol. 372, 129–147. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1986.sp016001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jahnsen, H. (1986b). Extracellular activation and membrane conductances of neurones in the guinea-pig deep cerebellar nuclei in vitro. J. Physiol. 372, 149–168. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1986.sp016002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jörntell, H., and Ekerot, C. F. (2002). Reciprocal bidirectional plasticity of parallel fiber receptive fields in cerebellar Purkinje cells and their afferent interneurons. Neuron 34, 797–806. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00713-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kassel, J., Shambes, G. M., and Welker, W. (1984). Fractured cutaneous projections to the granule cell layer of the posterior cerebellar hemisphere of the domestic cat. J. Comp. Neurol. 225, 458–468. doi: 10.1002/cne.902250311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kistler, W. M., and De Zeeuw, C. I. (2003). Time windows and reverberating loops: a reverse-engineering approach to cerebellar function. Cerebellum 2, 44–54. doi: 10.1080/14734220309426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koekkoek, S. K., Den Ouden, W. L., Perry, G., Highstein, S. M., and De Zeeuw, C. I. (2002). Monitoring kinetic and frequency-domain properties of eyelid responses in mice with magnetic distance measurement technique. J. Neurophysiol. 88, 2124–2133. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.88.4.2124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kruse, W., Krause, M., Aarse, J., Mark, M. D., Manahan-Vaughan, D., and Herlitze, S. (2014). Optogenetic modulation and multi-electrode analysis of cerebellar networks in vivo. PLoS One 9:e105589. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0105589

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LeDoux, M. S., Hurst, D. C., and Lorden, J. F. (1998). Single-unit activity of cerebellar nuclear cells in the awake genetically dystonic rat. Neuroscience 86, 533–545. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(98)00007-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Llinas, R., and Muhlethaler, M. (1988). An electrophysiological study of the in vitro, perfused brain stem-cerebellum of adult guinea-pig. J. Physiol. 404, 215–240. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1988.sp017287

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Llinas, R. R. (1988). The intrinsic electrophysiological properties of mammalian neurons: insights into central nervous system function. Science 242, 1654–1664. doi: 10.1126/science.3059497

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luque, N. R., Garrido, J. A., Carrillo, R. R., D’Angelo, E., and Ros, E. (2014). Fast convergence of learning requires plasticity between inferior olive and deep cerebellar nuclei in a manipulation task: a closed-loop robotic simulation. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 8:97. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2014.00097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mapelli, L., Gagliano, G., Soda, T., Laforenza, U., Moccia, F., and D’Angelo, E. U. (2017). Granular layer neurons control cerebellar neurovascular coupling through an NMDA receptor/NO-dependent system. J. Neurosci. 37, 1340–1351. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2025-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mapelli, L., Pagani, M., Garrido, J. A., and D’Angelo, E. (2015). Integrated plasticity at inhibitory and excitatory synapses in the cerebellar circuit. Front. Cell Neurosci. 9:169. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2015.00169

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marquez-Ruiz, J., and Cheron, G. (2012). Sensory stimulation-dependent plasticity in the cerebellar cortex of alert mice. PLoS One 7:e36184. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marr, D. (1969). A theory of cerebellar cortex. J. Physiol. 202, 437–470. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1969.sp008820

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshall, S. P., and Lang, E. J. (2004) Inferior olive oscillations gate transmission of motor cortical activity to the cerebellum. J. Neurosci. 24, 11356–11367. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3907-04.2004

CrossRef Full Text

Masoli, S., and D’Angelo, E. (2017). Synaptic activation of a detailed purkinje cell model predicts voltage-dependent control of burst-pause responses in active dendrites. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 11:278. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2017.00278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mawhinney, L. J., de Rivero Vaccari, J. P., Alonso, O. F., Jimenez, C. A., Furones, C., Moreno, W. J., et al. (2012). Isoflurane/nitrous oxide anesthesia induces increases in NMDA receptor subunit NR2B protein expression in the aged rat brain. Brain Res. 1431, 23–34. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Medina, J. F., Garcia, K. S., and Mauk, M. D. (2001). A mechanism for savings in the cerebellum. J. Neurosci. 21, 4081–4089. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-11-04081.2001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Medina, J. F., and Mauk, M. D. (1999). Simulations of cerebellar motor learning: computational analysis of plasticity at the mossy fiber to deep nucleus synapse. J. Neurosci. 19, 7140–7151. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-16-07140.1999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Medina, J. F., and Mauk, M. D. (2000). Computer simulation of cerebellar information processing. Nat. Neurosci. 3(Suppl.), 1205–1211. doi: 10.1038/81486

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mogensen, H., Bengtsson, F., and Jorntell, H. (2017). No medium-term spinocerebellar input plasticity in deep cerebellar nuclear neurons in vivo? Cerebellum 16, 638–647. doi: 10.1007/s12311-016-0839-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monaco, J., Casellato, C., Koch, G., and D’Angelo, E. (2014). Cerebellar theta burst stimulation dissociates memory components in eyeblink classical conditioning. Eur. J. Neurosci. 40, 3363–3370. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12700

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morcuende, S., Delgado-Garcia, J. M., and Ugolini, G. (2002). Neuronal premotor networks involved in eyelid responses: retrograde transneuronal tracing with rabies virus from the orbicularis oculi muscle in the rat. J. Neurosci. 22, 8808–8818. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-20-08808.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morishita, W., and Sastry, B. R. (1996). Postsynaptic mechanisms underlying long-term depression of GABAergic transmission in neurons of the deep cerebellar nuclei. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 59–68. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.76.1.59

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morissette, J., and Bower, J. M. (1996). Contribution of somatosensory cortex to responses in the rat cerebellar granule cell layer following peripheral tactile stimulation. Exp. Brain Res. 109, 240–250. doi: 10.1007/BF00231784

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muller, T., Grosche, J., Ohlemeyer, C., and Kettenmann, H. (1993). NMDA-activated currents in Bergmann glial cells. Neuroreport 4, 671–674. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199306000-00017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohyama, T., Nores, W. L., and Mauk, M. D. (2003). Stimulus generalization of conditioned eyelid responses produced without cerebellar cortex: implications for plasticity in the cerebellar nuclei. Learn. Mem. 10, 346–354. doi: 10.1101/lm.67103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohyama, T., Nores, W. L., Medina, J. F., Riusech, F. A., and Mauk, M. D. (2006). Learning-induced plasticity in deep cerebellar nucleus. J. Neurosci. 26, 12656–12663. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4023-06.2006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ouardouz, M., and Sastry, B. R. (2000). Mechanisms underlying LTP of inhibitory synaptic transmission in the deep cerebellar nuclei. J. Neurophysiol. 84, 1414–1421. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.84.3.1414

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prestori, F., Bonardi, C., Mapelli, L., Lombardo, P., Goselink, R., De Stefano, M. E., et al. (2013). Gating of long-term potentiation by nicotinic acetylcholine receptors at the cerebellum input stage. PLoS One 8:e64828. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0064828

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pugh, J. R., and Raman, I. M. (2006). Potentiation of mossy fiber EPSCs in the cerebellar nuclei by NMDA receptor activation followed by postinhibitory rebound current. Neuron 51, 113–123. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.05.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pugh, J. R., and Raman, I. M. (2009). Nothing can be coincidence: synaptic inhibition and plasticity in the cerebellar nuclei. Trends Neurosci. 32, 170–177. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Racine, R. J., Wilson, D. A., Gingell, R., and Sunderland, D. (1986). Long-term potentiation in the interpositus and vestibular nuclei in the rat. Exp. Brain Res. 63, 158–162. doi: 10.1007/BF00235658

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramakrishnan, K. B., Voges, K., De Propris, L., De Zeeuw, C. I., and D’Angelo, E. (2016) Tactile stimulation evokes long-lasting potentiation of purkinje cell discharge in vivo. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 10:36. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2016.00036

CrossRef Full Text

Raman, I. M., Gustafson, A. E., and Padgett, D. (2000). Ionic currents and spontaneous firing in neurons isolated from the cerebellar nuclei. J. Neurosci. 20, 9004–9016. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-24-09004.2000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roggeri, L., Rivieccio, B., Rossi, P., and D’Angelo, E. (2008). Tactile stimulation evokes long-term synaptic plasticity in the granular layer of cerebellum. J. Neurosci. 28, 6354–6359. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5709-07.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romano, V., De Propris, L., Bosman, L. W., Warnaar, P., Ten Brinke, M. M., Lindeman, S., et al. (2018). Potentiation of cerebellar Purkinje cells facilitates whisker reflex adaptation through increased simple spike activity. Elife 7:e38852. doi: 10.7554/eLife.38852

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rowland, N. C., and Jaeger, D. (2005). Coding of tactile response properties in the rat deep cerebellar nuclei. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 1236–1251. doi: 10.1152/jn.00285.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rowland, N. C., and Jaeger, D. (2008). Responses to tactile stimulation in deep cerebellar nucleus neurons result from recurrent activation in multiple pathways. J. Neurophysiol. 99, 704–717. doi: 10.1152/jn.01100.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roy, D., Sigala, R., Breakspear, M., McIntosh, A. R., Jirsa, V. K., Deco, G., et al. (2014). Using the virtual brain to reveal the role of oscillations and plasticity in shaping brain’s dynamical landscape. Brain Connect. 4, 791–811. doi: 10.1089/brain.2014.0252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sánchez-Campusano, R., Gruart, A., and Delgado-García, J. M. (2007). The cerebellar interpositus nucleus and the dynamic control of learned motor responses. J. Neurosci. 27, 6620–6632. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0488-07.2007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sánchez-Campusano, R., Gruart, A., and Delgado-García, J. M. (2009). Dynamic associations in the cerebellar-motoneuron network during motor learning. J. Neurosci. 29, 10750–10763. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2178-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schonewille, M., Belmeguenai, A., Koekkoek, S. K., Houtman, S. H., Boele, H. J., et al. (2010). Purkinje cell-specific knockout of the protein phosphatase PP2B impairs potentiation and cerebellar motor learning. Neuron 67, 618–628. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schweighofer, N., Doya, K., and Kuroda, S. (2004). Cerebellar aminergic neuromodulation: towards a functional understanding. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 44, 103–116. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2003.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shambes, G. M., Gibson, J. M., and Welker, W. (1978). Fractured somatotopy in granule cell tactile areas of rat cerebellar hemispheres revealed by micromapping. Brain Behav. Evol. 15, 94–140. doi: 10.1159/000123774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, M. A., Ghazizadeh, A., and Shadmehr, R. (2006). Interacting adaptive processes with different timescales underlie short-term motor learning. PLoS Biol. 4:e179. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solinas, S., Forti, L., Cesana, E., Mapelli, J., Schutter, E. D., and Angelo, E. D. (2007). Fast-reset of pacemaking and theta-frequency resonance patterns in cerebellar Golgi cells: simulations of their impact in vivo. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 1:4. doi: 10.3389/neuro.03.004.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sugihara, I., Lang, E. J., and Llinas, R. (1995). Serotonin modulation of inferior olivary oscillations and synchronicity: a multiple-electrode study in the rat cerebellum. Eur. J. Neurosci. 7, 521–534. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1995.tb00657.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sweeney, J. E., Lamour, Y., and Bassant, M. H. (1992). Arousal-dependent properties of medial septal neurons in the unanesthetized rat. Neuroscience 48, 353–362. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(92)90495-N

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Timofeev, I. (2011). Neuronal plasticity and thalamocortical sleep and waking oscillations. Prog. Brain Res. 193, 121–144. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53839-0.00009-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uusisaari, M. Y., and Knopfel, T. (2012). Diversity of neuronal elements and circuitry in the cerebellar nuclei. Cerebellum 11, 420–421. doi: 10.1007/s12311-011-0350-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valera, A. M., Binda, F., Pawlowski, S. A., Dupont, J. L., Casella, J. F., Rothstein, J. D., et al. (2016). Stereotyped spatial patterns of functional synaptic connectivity in the cerebellar cortex. Elife 5:e09862. doi: 10.7554/eLife.09862

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vos, B. P., Volny-Luraghi, A., and De Schutter, E. (1999). Cerebellar Golgi cells in the rat: receptive fields and timing of responses to facial stimulation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 11, 2621–2634. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00678.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, D., Smith-Bell, C. A., Burhans, L. B., O’Dell, D. E., Bell, R. W., and Schreurs, B. G. (2018). Changes in membrane properties of rat deep cerebellar nuclear projection neurons during acquisition of eyeblink conditioning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 115, E9419–E9428. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1808539115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, T. C., Becker, N., Apps, R., and Jones, M. W. (2014). Back to front: cerebellar connections and interactions with the prefrontal cortex. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:4. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Witter, L., Canto, C. B., Hoogland, T. M., de Gruijl, J. R., and De Zeeuw, C. I. (2013). Strength and timing of motor responses mediated by rebound firing in the cerebellar nuclei after Purkinje cell activation. Front. Neural Circuits 7:133. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yarden-Rabinowitz, Y., and Yarom, Y. (2017). In vivo analysis of synaptic activity in cerebellar nuclei neurons unravels the efficacy of excitatory inputs. J. Physiol. 595, 5945–5963. doi: 10.1113/JP274115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, G. J., Wu, M. C., Shi, J. D., Xu, Y. H., Chu, C. P., Cui, S. B., et al. (2017). Ethanol modulates the spontaneous complex spike waveform of cerebellar Purkinje Cells Recorded in vivo in Mice. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 11:43. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2017.00043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, W., and Linden, D. J. (2006). Long-term depression at the mossy fiber-deep cerebellar nucleus synapse. J. Neurosci. 26, 6935–6944. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0784-06.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, W., Shin, J. H., and Linden, D. J. (2004). Persistent changes in the intrinsic excitability of rat deep cerebellar nuclear neurones induced by EPSP or IPSP bursts. J. Physiol. 561, 703–719. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2004.071696

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: deep cerebellar nuclei, cerebellum, plasticity, oscillations, in vivo electrophysiology

Citation: Moscato L, Montagna I, De Propris L, Tritto S, Mapelli L and D’Angelo E (2019) Long-Lasting Response Changes in Deep Cerebellar Nuclei in vivo Correlate With Low-Frequency Oscillations. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 13:84. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2019.00084

Received: 30 October 2018; Accepted: 19 February 2019;
Published: 06 March 2019.

Edited by:

Sergey M. Korogod, Bogomoletz Institute of Physiology, National Academy of Sciences of Ukraine, Ukraine

Reviewed by:

Takashi Tominaga, Tokushima Bunri University, Japan
Mario U. Manto, University of Mons, Belgium

Copyright © 2019 Moscato, Montagna, De Propris, Tritto, Mapelli and D’Angelo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lisa Mapelli, lisa.mapelli@unipv.it Egidio D’Angelo, dangelo@unipv.it

Co-first authors

Co-last authors

Download